M. Drew LaMar
October 23, 2020
“There are no null results; there are only insufficiently clever choices of \( H_0 \). ”
- @richarddmorey
Definition: A
nonparametric method makes fewer assumptions than standardparametric methods do about the distribution of the variables.
Property: Nonparametric methods are usually based on the
ranks of the data points (medians, quartiles, etc.)
Property: Nonparametric tests are typically
less powerful than parametric tests.
Definition: The
sign test compares the median of a sample to a constant specified in the null hypothesis. It makes no assumptions about the distribution of the measurements in the population.
Definition: The
Mann-Whitney \( U \)-test compares the distributions of two groups. It does not require as many assumptions as the two-sample \( t \)-test.
Algorithm:
Sign test has very little power. If \( n \leq 5 \), then can't use sign test.
Assignment problem #25
Researchers have observed that rainforest areas next to clear-cuts (less than 100 meters away) have a reduced tree biomass compared to rainforest areas far from clear-cuts. To go further, Laurance et al. (1997) tested whether rainforest areas more distant from the clear-cuts were also affected. They compiled data on the biomass change after clear-cutting (in tons/hectare/year) for 36 rainforest areas between 100m and several kilometers from clear-cuts.
Look at data
ggplot(data = clearcuts) +
geom_histogram(aes(x = biomassChange),
color = "black", bins = 8)
hist(exp(clearcuts$biomassChange))
\( H_{0} \): The median change in biomass is zero.
\( H_{A} \): The median change in biomass is not zero.
# Any biomass equal to zero?
sum(clearcuts$biomassChange == 0)
[1] 0
# How many plots have positive change in biomass?
(X <- sum(clearcuts$biomassChange > 0))
[1] 21
# Perform binomial test
binom.test(X, n=36, p=0.5)
Exact binomial test
data: X and 36
number of successes = 21, number of trials = 36, p-value = 0.405
alternative hypothesis: true probability of success is not equal to 0.5
95 percent confidence interval:
0.4075652 0.7448590
sample estimates:
probability of success
0.5833333
Definition: The
Mann-Whitney \( U \)-test compares the distributions of two groups. It does not require as many assumptions as the two-sample \( t \)-test.
Assignment Problem #32
T and B lymphocytes are normal components of the immune system, but in multiple sclerosis they become autoreactive and attack the central nervous system. What triggers the autoimmune process? One hypothesis is that the disease is initiated by environmental factors, especially microbial infection. However, recent work by Berer et al. (2011) on the mouse model of the disease suggests that the autoimmune process is triggered by nonpathogenic microbes living in the gut.
They compared onset of autoimmune encephalomyelitis in two treatment groups of mice from a strain that carries transgenic human CD4\( ^{+} \) T cells, which initiate the disease. One group (GF) was kept free of nonpathogenic gut microbes and all pathogens. The other (SPF) was only pathogen-free and served as controls. They measured percentage of T cells producing the molecule, interleukin-17, in tissue samples from 16 mice in the two groups.
Look at the data
treatment percentInterleukin17
1 SPF 18.87
2 SPF 15.65
3 SPF 13.45
4 SPF 12.95
5 SPF 6.01
6 SPF 5.84
Discuss: Is this data in tidy or messy format?
Answer: Tidy
Look at data
Discuss: Discuss the data with respect to meeting assumptions of statistical tests.
Look at data
Look at data
mydata %>%
ggplot(aes(x = percentInterleukin17)) +
geom_histogram(binwidth=4) +
facet_grid(treatment ~ .) +
xlab("Percent interleukin-17") +
ylab("Frequency")
Mann-Whitney \( U \)-test (Wilcoxon rank-sum test)
\( H_{0} \): The distribution of interleukin-17 is the same in the two groups.
\( H_{A} \): The distribution of interleukin-17 is NOT the same in the two groups.
wilcox.test(percentInterleukin17 ~ treatment, data = mydata)
Mann-Whitney \( U \)-test (Wilcoxon rank-sum test)
Wilcoxon rank sum exact test
data: percentInterleukin17 by treatment
W = 6, p-value = 0.004662
alternative hypothesis: true location shift is not equal to 0
Conclusion?
The Mann-Whitney \( U \)-test tests if the distributions are the same.
If the distributions of the two groups have the same shape (same variance and skew), then the Mann-Whitney \( U \)-test can be used to compare the locations (means or medians) of the two groups (see Example 13.5).
It is for this reason that this test gets misused a lot in the literature.
Goal: Understand variation
We want to distinguish between variation of interest and variation from other sources (again, increase signal-to-noise ratio).
“Whenever we carry out an experiment or observational study, we are either interested in measuring random variation, or (more often) trying to find ways to remove or reduce the effects of random variation, so that the effects that we care about can be seen more clearly.”
Definition:
Replication involves making the same manipulations to and taking the same measurements on a number of different, independent experimental subjects.
We are essentially talking about sample size here, but there is more to it than that due to the independence issue.
http://www.zoology.ubc.ca/~whitlock/kingfisher/SamplingNormal.htm
Example: Does sex have an effect on human height?
Measure height in 10 married couples of the opposite sex. Are we safe in restricting our sample to married couples?
Definition:
Pseudoreplicates are dependent measures.
Definition: Replicate measures must be
independent of each other, i.e. a measurement made on one individual should not provide any useful information about that factor on another individual.
Definition:
Pseudoreplication occurs if we analyze pseudoreplicates as if they were replicates.
When we pseudoreplicate, we are making a false claim about the amount of replication.
Both accuracy and precision are affected by pseudoreplication!
Accuracy: Pseudoreplication changes our question from general and interesting to more specific and less interesting.
Precision: Pseudoreplication underestimates the precision due to dependence of measures of interest.
Question: Do blue tit nestlings raised in nest boxes suffer more from external parasites than those raised in natural cavities?
Experimental Design: Investigate the four nestlings in a particular nest box and count the number of parasites on each.
Discuss: Why are these pseudoreplicates? Explain how they affect precision and accuracy.
Answer #1: This design gives you information on parasite load
only for birds in this particular nest box .
Answer #2: Nestlings will be similar in many ways due to sharing nest box, and thus
variation of parasite load between nestlings in this box will be smaller than between all nestlings.
Pseudoreplication is a biological and experimental design issue, not a statistical issue. Data doesn't look pseudoreplicated.
Common sources of pseudoreplication:
Make sure you are…
Experimental study: Make sure individuals differ systematically only in the explanatory variable(s) of interest.
Observational studies: Be aware of confounding variables.
Random sampling, or randomization, can solve many of these problems.
Blocking is another technique to address pseudoreplication in experimental studies.
Matching is analogous to blocking for observational studies.
Birdsongs and attractiveness
Question: How do we measure relationship between male birdsongs and attractiveness to females?
Experimental Design: Record the complex song of one male and the simple song of another male, and then play these same two songs to each of 40 different females. Compute a confidence interval for the mean attractiveness of the two male songs.
Discuss: What is wrong with this design so far?
Answer: Each measure of female choice is a pseudoreplicate (\( n=40 \)).
Discuss: What is wrong with this design so far?
Answer: Each measure of female choice is a pseudoreplicate (\( n=40 \)).
Discuss: What can we do to correct for this pseudoreplication?
Answer: Record songs of 40 males with complex songs, and 40 separate males with simple songs. Each female should listen to a unique pair of songs, one simple and one complex. Design can get even more complicated than this.
Discuss: What are examples of confounding variables in the pseudoreplicated case?
Blood sugar levels
Experimental Design: Phlebotomist takes 15 samples from each of 10 patients, yielding a total of 150 measurements.
Discuss: What is the replicate and sample size in this situation? Why?
Antibiotics and bacterial growth rates
Experimental Design: Two agar plates: one with antibiotic, one without. Spread bacteria on both plates, let them grow for 24 hours, then measure diameter of 100 colonies on each plate?
Discuss: What is the replicate and sample size in this situation? Why?
Three things:
We'll use a two-sample \( t \)-test as the example in this section.
We would like to compute a 95% confidence interval for \( \mu_{1}-\mu_{2} \).
\[ \bar{Y}_{1}-\bar{Y}_{2} \pm \mathrm{margin \ of \ error}, \]
where “margin of error” is the half-width of the 95% confidence interval.
In this case, the following formula is an approximation to the number of samples needed to achieve the desired margin of error (assuming balanced design, i.e. \( n_{1}=n_{2}=n \)):
\[ n \approx 8\left(\frac{\mathrm{margin \ of \ error}}{\sigma}\right)^{-2} \]
Two-sample \( t \)-test:
\[ H_{0}: \mu_{1} - \mu_{2} = 0. \] \[ H_{A}: \mu_{1} - \mu_{2} \neq 0. \]
A conventional power to aim for is 0.80, i.e. we aim to prove \( H_{0} \) is false in 80% of experiments.
Assuming a significance level of 0.05, a quick approximation to the planned sample size \( n \) in each of two groups is
\[ n \approx 16\left(\frac{D}{\sigma}\right)^{-2}, \]
where \( D = |\mu_{1}-\mu_{2}| \) is the effect size.
library(pwr)
function | power calculations for |
---|---|
pwr.2p.test | two proportions (equal n) |
pwr.2p2n.test | two proportions (unequal n) |
pwr.anova.test | balanced one way ANOVA |
pwr.chisq.test | chi-square test |
pwr.f2.test | general linear model |
pwr.p.test | proportion (one sample) |
pwr.r.test | correlation |
pwr.t.test | t-tests (one sample, 2 sample, paired) |
pwr.t2n.test | t-test (two samples with unequal n) |
Two-sample \( t \)-test with significance level 0.05, 80% power, and relative effect size \( d = \frac{|\mu_{1}-\mu_{2}|}{\sigma} = 0.3 \).
pwr.t.test(d=0.3, power=0.8, type="two.sample")
Two-sample t test power calculation
n = 175.3847
d = 0.3
sig.level = 0.05
power = 0.8
alternative = two.sided
NOTE: n is number in *each* group
Definition: Proper
randomization means that any individual experimental subject has the same chance as any other individual of finding itself in each experimental group, as well as prepared, setup, or measured in the same way.
Definition: Proper
randomization means that any individual experimental subject has the same chance as any other individual of finding itself in each experimental group, as well as prepared, setup, or measured in the same way.
Question: Does a specific genetic modification to a tomato plant affect its growth rate?
Experimental Design: Place 50 genetically modified plants, and 50 unmodified plants, into individual pots with compost, and then put them all into a growth chamber.
Discuss: Where can improper randomization appear in this example?
Answer: For example:
- Difference in compost quality.
- Difference in temperature across chamber.
Let's look at temperature as a possible confounding variable:
The above randomization would not remove temperature difference across chamber, but simply remove correlation with treatment.
What if we would like to reduce the variation from temperature? We can try blocking.
Our attempt to control for temperature:
Discuss: What’s right and wrong with this particular design?
This particular blocking design is properly replicated and randomized.
The variation due to temperature in each chamber has been reduced, so that the difference between treatments becomes more apparent.
There was a systematic difference of temperature across the original chamber. We have now adjusted the design to systematically account for this difference.
What if you can't do experiments? Randomization does not apply here.
Two strategies are used to limit effects of confounding variables on a difference between treatments in a controlled observational study.
Definition: With
matching , every individual in the treatment group is paired with a control individual having the same of closely similar values for the suspected confounding variable.
Definition: With
adjustment , use a statistical method, such asanalysis of covariance , to correct for differences between treatment and control groups in suspected confounding variables.
Assigning treatments to subjects (one possibility):
Remember, randomization is important in all processes of the experiment, including preparation, setup, and measurement.
Randomize measurement of replicates in time:
This shows time of measurement could be a confounding factor.