Many college courses conclude by giving students the opportunity to evaluate the course and the instructor anonymously. However, the use of these student evaluations as an indicator of course quality and teaching effectiveness is often criticized because these measures may reflect the influence of non-teaching related characteristics, such as the physical appearance of the instructor. The article titled, “Beauty in the classroom: instructors’ pulchritude and putative pedagogical productivity” (Hamermesh and Parker, 2005) found that instructors who are viewed to be better looking receive higher instructional ratings. (Daniel S. Hamermesh, Amy Parker, Beauty in the classroom: instructors pulchritude and putative pedagogical productivity, Economics of Education Review, Volume 24, Issue 4, August 2005, Pages 369-376, ISSN 0272-7757, 10.1016/j.econedurev.2004.07.013. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0272775704001165.)
In this lab we will analyze the data from this study in order to learn what goes into a positive professor evaluation.
The data were gathered from end of semester student evaluations for a large sample of professors from the University of Texas at Austin. In addition, six students rated the professors’ physical appearance. (This is aslightly modified version of the original data set that was released as part of the replication data for Data Analysis Using Regression and Multilevel/Hierarchical Models (Gelman and Hill, 2007).) The result is a data frame where each row contains a different course and columns represent variables about the courses and professors.
load("more/evals.RData")
variable | description |
---|---|
score |
average professor evaluation score: (1) very unsatisfactory - (5) excellent. |
rank |
rank of professor: teaching, tenure track, tenured. |
ethnicity |
ethnicity of professor: not minority, minority. |
gender |
gender of professor: female, male. |
language |
language of school where professor received education: english or non-english. |
age |
age of professor. |
cls_perc_eval |
percent of students in class who completed evaluation. |
cls_did_eval |
number of students in class who completed evaluation. |
cls_students |
total number of students in class. |
cls_level |
class level: lower, upper. |
cls_profs |
number of professors teaching sections in course in sample: single, multiple. |
cls_credits |
number of credits of class: one credit (lab, PE, etc.), multi credit. |
bty_f1lower |
beauty rating of professor from lower level female: (1) lowest - (10) highest. |
bty_f1upper |
beauty rating of professor from upper level female: (1) lowest - (10) highest. |
bty_f2upper |
beauty rating of professor from second upper level female: (1) lowest - (10) highest. |
bty_m1lower |
beauty rating of professor from lower level male: (1) lowest - (10) highest. |
bty_m1upper |
beauty rating of professor from upper level male: (1) lowest - (10) highest. |
bty_m2upper |
beauty rating of professor from second upper level male: (1) lowest - (10) highest. |
bty_avg |
average beauty rating of professor. |
pic_outfit |
outfit of professor in picture: not formal, formal. |
pic_color |
color of professor’s picture: color, black & white. |
Is this an observational study or an experiment? The original research question posed in the paper is whether beauty leads directly to the differences in course evaluations. Given the study design, is it possible to answer this question as it is phrased? If not, rephrase the question.
This is an observational study. We should rephrase the question to “Does beauty of professors influence students’ course evaluations?”
Describe the distribution of score
. Is the distribution skewed? What does that tell you about how students rate courses? Is this what you expected to see? Why, or why not?
hist(evals$score)
#score is left skewed, so most students rated their courses highly. I expected the distribution to be more normal.
Excluding score
, select two other variables and describe their relationship using an appropriate visualization (scatterplot, side-by-side boxplots, or mosaic plot).
library(ggplot2)
ggplot() +
geom_boxplot(mapping = aes(x = evals$gender, y = evals$age))
#Males have a larger spread in age than females. The mean age for females is about 46 while the mor males is about 51.
The fundamental phenomenon suggested by the study is that better looking teachers are evaluated more favorably. Let’s create a scatterplot to see if this appears to be the case:
plot(evals$score ~ evals$bty_avg)
Before we draw conclusions about the trend, compare the number of observations in the data frame with the approximate number of points on the scatterplot. Is anything awry?
Replot the scatterplot, but this time use the function jitter()
on the \(y\)- or the \(x\)-coordinate. (Use ?jitter
to learn more.) What was misleading about the initial scatterplot?
plot(jitter(evals$score) ~ evals$bty_avg)
#The initial scatterplot was misleading because it displayed fewer points than we have observations. By using jitter(), we see that multiple observations have the same coordinates so they were plotted on top of one another.
Let’s see if the apparent trend in the plot is something more than natural variation. Fit a linear model called m_bty
to predict average professor score by average beauty rating and add the line to your plot using abline(m_bty)
. Write out the equation for the linear model and interpret the slope. Is average beauty score a statistically significant predictor? Does it appear to be a practically significant predictor?
m_bty <- lm(evals$score ~ evals$bty_avg)
plot(jitter(evals$score) ~ evals$bty_avg)
abline(m_bty)
summary(m_bty)
##
## Call:
## lm(formula = evals$score ~ evals$bty_avg)
##
## Residuals:
## Min 1Q Median 3Q Max
## -1.9246 -0.3690 0.1420 0.3977 0.9309
##
## Coefficients:
## Estimate Std. Error t value Pr(>|t|)
## (Intercept) 3.88034 0.07614 50.96 < 2e-16 ***
## evals$bty_avg 0.06664 0.01629 4.09 5.08e-05 ***
## ---
## Signif. codes: 0 '***' 0.001 '**' 0.01 '*' 0.05 '.' 0.1 ' ' 1
##
## Residual standard error: 0.5348 on 461 degrees of freedom
## Multiple R-squared: 0.03502, Adjusted R-squared: 0.03293
## F-statistic: 16.73 on 1 and 461 DF, p-value: 5.083e-05
#The slope indicates that for each increase in average beauty rating, score increases by 0.06664.
#Beauty score is a statistically significant predictor since its p-value is 5.08e-05
\[ \hat{y} = 3.88034 + 0.06664 \times average beauty score \]
Use residual plots to evaluate whether the conditions of least squares regression are reasonable. Provide plots and comments for each one (see the Simple Regression Lab for a reminder of how to make these).
plot(m_bty$residuals ~ evals$bty_avg)
abline(h = 0, lty = 3)
#There is no pattern in the residuals plot so the data meets the linearity assumption
The data set contains several variables on the beauty score of the professor: individual ratings from each of the six students who were asked to score the physical appearance of the professors and the average of these six scores. Let’s take a look at the relationship between one of these scores and the average beauty score.
plot(evals$bty_avg ~ evals$bty_f1lower)
cor(evals$bty_avg, evals$bty_f1lower)
As expected the relationship is quite strong - after all, the average score is calculated using the individual scores. We can actually take a look at the relationships between all beauty variables (columns 13 through 19) using the following command:
plot(evals[,13:19])
These variables are collinear (correlated), and adding more than one of these variables to the model would not add much value to the model. In this application and with these highly-correlated predictors, it is reasonable to use the average beauty score as the single representative of these variables.
In order to see if beauty is still a significant predictor of professor score after we’ve accounted for the gender of the professor, we can add the gender term into the model.
m_bty_gen <- lm(score ~ bty_avg + gender, data = evals)
summary(m_bty_gen)
#qqnorm(m_bty_gen$residuals)
#qqline(m_bty_gen$residuals)
#plot(abs(m_bty_gen$residuals) ~ m_bty_gen$fitted.values)
#plot(m_bty_gen$residuals ~ evals$gender)
#The model meets all assumtpions
Is bty_avg
still a significant predictor of score
? Has the addition of gender
to the model changed the parameter estimate for bty_avg
?
Yes, bty_avg
is still a significant predictor of score
. The estimate is now 0.07416 so the addition of gender
to the model did change the parameter estimate for bty_avg
Note that the estimate for gender
is now called gendermale
. You’ll see this name change whenever you introduce a categorical variable. The reason is that R recodes gender
from having the values of female
and male
to being an indicator variable called gendermale
that takes a value of \(0\) for females and a value of \(1\) for males. (Such variables are often referred to as “dummy” variables.)
As a result, for females, the parameter estimate is multiplied by zero, leaving the intercept and slope form familiar from simple regression.
\[ \begin{aligned} \widehat{score} &= \hat{\beta}_0 + \hat{\beta}_1 \times bty\_avg + \hat{\beta}_2 \times (0) \\ &= \hat{\beta}_0 + \hat{\beta}_1 \times bty\_avg\end{aligned} \]
We can plot this line and the line corresponding to males with the following custom function.
multiLines(m_bty_gen)
What is the equation of the line corresponding to males? (Hint: For males, the parameter estimate is multiplied by 1.) For two professors who received the same beauty rating, which gender tends to have the higher course evaluation score? \[ \hat{y} = 3.74734 + 0.07416 \times bty_avg + 0.17239 \times 1 \]
For two professors who received the same beauty rating, males have the higher course evaluation score.
The decision to call the indicator variable gendermale
instead ofgenderfemale
has no deeper meaning. R simply codes the category that comes first alphabetically as a \(0\). (You can change the reference level of a categorical variable, which is the level that is coded as a 0, using therelevel
function. Use ?relevel
to learn more.)
Create a new model called m_bty_rank
with gender
removed and rank
added in. How does R appear to handle categorical variables that have more than two levels? Note that the rank variable has three levels: teaching
, tenure track
, tenured
.
m_bty_rank <- lm(score ~ bty_avg + rank, data = evals)
summary(m_bty_rank)
##
## Call:
## lm(formula = score ~ bty_avg + rank, data = evals)
##
## Residuals:
## Min 1Q Median 3Q Max
## -1.8713 -0.3642 0.1489 0.4103 0.9525
##
## Coefficients:
## Estimate Std. Error t value Pr(>|t|)
## (Intercept) 3.98155 0.09078 43.860 < 2e-16 ***
## bty_avg 0.06783 0.01655 4.098 4.92e-05 ***
## ranktenure track -0.16070 0.07395 -2.173 0.0303 *
## ranktenured -0.12623 0.06266 -2.014 0.0445 *
## ---
## Signif. codes: 0 '***' 0.001 '**' 0.01 '*' 0.05 '.' 0.1 ' ' 1
##
## Residual standard error: 0.5328 on 459 degrees of freedom
## Multiple R-squared: 0.04652, Adjusted R-squared: 0.04029
## F-statistic: 7.465 on 3 and 459 DF, p-value: 6.88e-05
#R makes the 3 levels into one dummy variable and two dichotomous variables
The interpretation of the coefficients in multiple regression is slightly different from that of simple regression. The estimate for bty_avg
reflects how much higher a group of professors is expected to score if they have a beauty rating that is one point higher while holding all other variables constant. In this case, that translates into considering only professors of the same rank with bty_avg
scores that are one point apart.
We will start with a full model that predicts professor score based on rank, ethnicity, gender, language of the university where they got their degree, age, proportion of students that filled out evaluations, class size, course level, number of professors, number of credits, average beauty rating, outfit, and picture color.
Which variable would you expect to have the highest p-value in this model? Why? Hint: Think about which variable would you expect to not have any association with the professor score.
I would expect cls_students
to have the highest p-value in this model because I don’t think it has high association with the course evaluation score
Let’s run the model…
m_full <- lm(score ~ rank + ethnicity + gender + language + age + cls_perc_eval
+ cls_students + cls_level + cls_profs + cls_credits + bty_avg
+ pic_outfit + pic_color, data = evals)
summary(m_full)
Check your suspicions from the previous exercise. Include the model output in your response.
The estimate for ‘cls_profs’ has the largest p-value of 0.77806
Interpret the coefficient associated with the ethnicity variable.
With all other variables remaining constant, score increases by 0.1234929 if the professor is not a minority.
Drop the variable with the highest p-value and re-fit the model. Did the coefficients and significance of the other explanatory variables change? (One of the things that makes multiple regression interesting is that coefficient estimates depend on the other variables that are included in the model.) If not, what does this say about whether or not the dropped variable was collinear with the other explanatory variables?
m_full2 <- lm(score ~ rank + ethnicity + gender + language + age + cls_perc_eval
+ cls_students + cls_level + cls_credits + bty_avg
+ pic_outfit + pic_color, data = evals)
summary(m_full2)
##
## Call:
## lm(formula = score ~ rank + ethnicity + gender + language + age +
## cls_perc_eval + cls_students + cls_level + cls_credits +
## bty_avg + pic_outfit + pic_color, data = evals)
##
## Residuals:
## Min 1Q Median 3Q Max
## -1.7836 -0.3257 0.0859 0.3513 0.9551
##
## Coefficients:
## Estimate Std. Error t value Pr(>|t|)
## (Intercept) 4.0872523 0.2888562 14.150 < 2e-16 ***
## ranktenure track -0.1476746 0.0819824 -1.801 0.072327 .
## ranktenured -0.0973829 0.0662614 -1.470 0.142349
## ethnicitynot minority 0.1274458 0.0772887 1.649 0.099856 .
## gendermale 0.2101231 0.0516873 4.065 5.66e-05 ***
## languagenon-english -0.2282894 0.1111305 -2.054 0.040530 *
## age -0.0089992 0.0031326 -2.873 0.004262 **
## cls_perc_eval 0.0052888 0.0015317 3.453 0.000607 ***
## cls_students 0.0004687 0.0003737 1.254 0.210384
## cls_levelupper 0.0606374 0.0575010 1.055 0.292200
## cls_creditsone credit 0.5061196 0.1149163 4.404 1.33e-05 ***
## bty_avg 0.0398629 0.0174780 2.281 0.023032 *
## pic_outfitnot formal -0.1083227 0.0721711 -1.501 0.134080
## pic_colorcolor -0.2190527 0.0711469 -3.079 0.002205 **
## ---
## Signif. codes: 0 '***' 0.001 '**' 0.01 '*' 0.05 '.' 0.1 ' ' 1
##
## Residual standard error: 0.4974 on 449 degrees of freedom
## Multiple R-squared: 0.187, Adjusted R-squared: 0.1634
## F-statistic: 7.943 on 13 and 449 DF, p-value: 2.336e-14
The coefficients and significance of the other explanatory variables did not change much after dropping ‘cls_prof’. So ’cls_prof is not collinear with the other explanatory variables.
m_best <- lm(score ~ ethnicity + gender + language + age + cls_perc_eval
+ cls_credits + bty_avg + pic_color, data = evals)
summary(m_best)
##
## Call:
## lm(formula = score ~ ethnicity + gender + language + age + cls_perc_eval +
## cls_credits + bty_avg + pic_color, data = evals)
##
## Residuals:
## Min 1Q Median 3Q Max
## -1.85320 -0.32394 0.09984 0.37930 0.93610
##
## Coefficients:
## Estimate Std. Error t value Pr(>|t|)
## (Intercept) 3.771922 0.232053 16.255 < 2e-16 ***
## ethnicitynot minority 0.167872 0.075275 2.230 0.02623 *
## gendermale 0.207112 0.050135 4.131 4.30e-05 ***
## languagenon-english -0.206178 0.103639 -1.989 0.04726 *
## age -0.006046 0.002612 -2.315 0.02108 *
## cls_perc_eval 0.004656 0.001435 3.244 0.00127 **
## cls_creditsone credit 0.505306 0.104119 4.853 1.67e-06 ***
## bty_avg 0.051069 0.016934 3.016 0.00271 **
## pic_colorcolor -0.190579 0.067351 -2.830 0.00487 **
## ---
## Signif. codes: 0 '***' 0.001 '**' 0.01 '*' 0.05 '.' 0.1 ' ' 1
##
## Residual standard error: 0.4992 on 454 degrees of freedom
## Multiple R-squared: 0.1722, Adjusted R-squared: 0.1576
## F-statistic: 11.8 on 8 and 454 DF, p-value: 2.58e-15
\[ \widehat{score} = 3.771922 + 167872 \times ethnicity + 0.207112 \times gender -0.206178 \times language -0.006046 \times age + 0.004656 \times cls_perc_eval + 0.505306 \times cls_credits + 0.051069 \times bty_avg -0.190579 \times pic_color \]
#qqnorm(m_best$residuals)
#qqline(m_best$residuals)
#plot(abs(m_bty_gen$residuals) ~ m_bty_gen$fitted.values)
#The model meets all assumptions
The original paper describes how these data were gathered by taking a sample of professors from the University of Texas at Austin and including all courses that they have taught. Considering that each row represents a course, could this new information have an impact on any of the conditions of linear regression?
This new information may influence the independence assumption
Based on your final model, describe the characteristics of a professor and course at University of Texas at Austin that would be associated with a high evaluation score.
Based on the model, the highest score will be associated with professors who:
1. Is not a minority
2. Male
3. Received education at an English speaking school
4. Young
5. Had a high percentage of students fill out evaluations
6. Teaches one credit courses
7. Has a high beauty ranking
8. Has a black & white picture
Would you be comfortable generalizing your conclusions to apply to professors generally (at any university)? Why or why not?
Assuming that this obersvational study has random sampling but no random assignment, I would be comfortable generalizing my conclusions to apply to professors generally.
This is a product of OpenIntro that is released under a Creative Commons Attribution-ShareAlike 3.0 Unported. This lab was written by Mine Çetinkaya-Rundel and Andrew Bray.