How Do We-words Moderate the Impact of Positive and Negative Mood States and Relationship Dynamics/Behaviors on Conflict Intensity in Couple Conflicts?
Couple Conflict and Intensity
Conflicts reveal couples’ communication patterns, conflict resolution approaches, and emotional closeness.1
Intense conflicts can elevate levels of stress, anxiety, and depression among couples, affecting their psychological well-being.2
Negative Moods
Criticism, stress, and defensiveness are predictors of increased conflict and marital dissolution.3
Negative affect often escalates conflicts and leads to long-term relationship dissatisfaction.4
Positive Relationship Dynamics/Behaviors
Positive affect mitigates negative moods/behaviors and buffers against conflict escalation. 5
Maintaining a balance where positive interactions outweigh negative interactions is crucial for effective conflict management.6
We-Words
We-words are associated with elevated positive emotional behavior and reduced negative emotional behavior and play a role in regulating partners’ emotions during marital conflicts.7
As reflected in “we” language, perspective-taking diminishes conflict intensity by fostering empathy, promoting adaptive resolution strategies, and enhancing mutual understanding in relationships.8
The sample had 25% of participants identified as Caucasian, 24.3% Hispanic/Latino, 18.4% African American, 11% Asian, 19.1% Multiracial, and 2.21% Other
Measures
Couples completed an at-home study using smartphones to provide 3- minute audio recordings every 6 minutes throughout the day.
Audio files were listened to, and conflict episodes were identified (n = 68 files).
One 3-minute conflict audio recording was selected per couple, with each partner rated on 13 positive mood/relationship dynamics, 20 negative mood/relationship dynamics, and conflict intensity every second.
The conflict audio files were transcribed.
Individual positive behaviors (Daily Business, Collaborate, Enjoy) and negative moods (Anxiety/Irritation, Stress, Anger/Hostility) were averaged to represent overall positive behaviors and negative mood, respectively.
Analysis
Load packages and read data
View Code
library(tidyverse) # Data manipulation and visualizationlibrary(dplyr) # Data wranglinglibrary(officer) # Create/edit Word documentslibrary(pdp) # Partial dependence plotslibrary(stringr) # String manipulation (for tokenization)library(interactions) # Plot interaction effects in modelslibrary(lme4) # Mixed-effects modelslibrary(lmerTest) # p-values for fixed effects in mixed modelslibrary(MuMIn) # Model selection and evaluation (AICc, R², model averaging)library(prettydoc)data <-read_csv("../USC/USC_clean/USC-Coded-Data-Masterfile.csv")
Preview Data
View Code
data |>select(-c(seconds, Date_Coded))
Data Wrangling
Replaced placeholder values with NA in numeric columns
View Code
# Replace placeholder values (222, 999 and 444) with NA in numeric columns excluding 'Couple ID'data_cleaned <- data |>mutate(across(everything(), ~ifelse(. %in%c(999, 444, 222), NA, .))) |># rename sad and whiny column to match the rest of the column namesrename(P1Sad = P1_Sad, P1Whiny = P1_Whiny, P2Sad = P2_Sad, P2Whiny = P2_Whiny) |># create new variable 'overall_conflict' to represent conflict intensity for both partners# accounts for coding inconsistencies between P1Conflict and P2Conflictmutate(overall_conflict = (P1Conflict + P2Conflict)/2)data_cleaned |>select(-c(seconds, Date_Coded)) |>head(10)
Based on research, I identified 4 positive and negative moods/behaviors respectively in representing the overall positive and negative moods. Below I combined the positive behaviors together and combined the negative moods together.
The values were log transformed for running the multiple regression model.
View Code
scaled_vars <- data_cleaned |>## average the variables to combine themrowwise() |># calculates mean across row# Partner 1mutate(P1PosBehv =mean(c(P1Collab, P1DailyB, P1Enjoy), na.rm =TRUE)) |>mutate(P1NegMood =mean(c(P1AnnIrr, P1AngHos, P1Stress), na.rm =TRUE)) |># Partner 2mutate(P2PosBehv =mean(c(P2Collab, P2DailyB, P2Enjoy), na.rm =TRUE)) |>mutate(P2NegMood =mean(c(P2AnnIrr, P2AngHos, P2Stress), na.rm =TRUE)) |>## log transformationmutate(P1PosBehv =log(P1PosBehv +1), # Adding 1 to handle zero values, if anyP1NegMood =log(P1NegMood +1),P2PosBehv =log(P2PosBehv +1),P2NegMood =log(P2NegMood +1),overall_conflict =log(overall_conflict +1)) |># select the variables for regression analysisselect(c(ID, Time, min_sec, overall_conflict:P2NegMood))scaled_vars |>head(10)
Text Analysis
Loading the transcript files, and processed each word file to get the frequency of “we-words” in each document, then I included it to the tibble above with the positive and negative moods/behaviors. To simplify the code for multiple regression model, I created variables defining the interaction terms beforehand.
View Code
# define my DOCX filesdocx_files <-c("../USC/Transcripts/102-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/104-3-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/124-2 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/126-2 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/133-3-2 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/135-8-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/156-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/188-3-2 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/204-8-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/206-3-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/707-2 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/710-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/712-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/713-2 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/714-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/717-2 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/720-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/721-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/722-2 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/724-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/725-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/727-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/730-2 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/732-2 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/733-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/735-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/736-2 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/739-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/740-2 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/741-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/744-2 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/745-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/747-2 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/749-2 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/750-2 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/751-2 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/752-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/753-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/756-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/757-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/758-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/759-2 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/760-2 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/762-2 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/763-2.docx","../USC/Transcripts/767-2.docx","../USC/Transcripts/768-2 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/769-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/771-1.docx","../USC/Transcripts/773-2 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/907-2 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/908-8-2 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/924-2 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/930-8-1 LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/945-8-2LIWC.docx","../USC/Transcripts/966-3-1 LIWC.docx")# function to read and process each DOCX fileread_and_process_docx <-function(file_path) { doc <-read_docx(file_path) text <-docx_summary(doc)$texttibble(transcript =basename(file_path),text = text )}# read and process all DOCX filestranscript_df <- purrr::map_df(docx_files, read_and_process_docx)# tokenize texttokens <- transcript_df %>%mutate(tokens =str_extract_all(text, "\\b\\w+\\b")) %>%select(transcript, tokens) %>%unnest(tokens) |>mutate(ID =as.numeric(str_extract(transcript, "^\\d{3}")))# count occurrences of we-wordswe_counts <- tokens %>%filter(tokens %in%c("we")) %>%count(ID, word = tokens) |># rearrange the tablepivot_wider(names_from = word,values_from = n)################################################## combine the we-word counts to the moods/behaviors tibbleall_vars <- scaled_vars |>left_join(we_counts, by ="ID") |>mutate(we =as.numeric(we))# remove NAsall_vars_clean <- all_vars |>drop_na()# Create interaction terms in the dataall_vars_withInteract <- all_vars_clean %>%mutate(P1P2_PosInteraction = P1PosBehv * P2PosBehv,P1P2_NegInteraction = P1NegMood * P2NegMood,P1Pos_P2Neg_Interaction = P1PosBehv * P2NegMood,P1Neg_P2Pos_Interaction = P1NegMood * P2PosBehv)all_vars_withInteract |>head(10)
Regression Analysis
View Code
lm_model <-lmer(overall_conflict ~ P1P2_PosInteraction * we + P1P2_NegInteraction * we + P1Pos_P2Neg_Interaction * we + P1Neg_P2Pos_Interaction * we + P1PosBehv * P2PosBehv * we + P1NegMood* P2NegMood * we + P1PosBehv * P2NegMood * we + P1NegMood * P2PosBehv * we + (1| ID), data = all_vars_withInteract)summary(lm_model)
Linear mixed model fit by REML. t-tests use Satterthwaite's method [
lmerModLmerTest]
Formula: overall_conflict ~ P1P2_PosInteraction * we + P1P2_NegInteraction *
we + P1Pos_P2Neg_Interaction * we + P1Neg_P2Pos_Interaction *
we + P1PosBehv * P2PosBehv * we + P1NegMood * P2NegMood *
we + P1PosBehv * P2NegMood * we + P1NegMood * P2PosBehv *
we + (1 | ID)
Data: all_vars_withInteract
REML criterion at convergence: 417.1
Scaled residuals:
Min 1Q Median 3Q Max
-3.6495 -0.4851 0.0759 0.4491 3.6940
Random effects:
Groups Name Variance Std.Dev.
ID (Intercept) 0.2891 0.5377
Residual 0.1801 0.4243
Number of obs: 257, groups: ID, 25
Fixed effects:
Estimate Std. Error df t value Pr(>|t|)
(Intercept) 0.28491 0.24809 54.64659 1.148 0.25580
P1P2_PosInteraction -0.25726 0.13446 230.04242 -1.913 0.05695 .
we -0.01096 0.08363 59.55009 -0.131 0.89615
P1P2_NegInteraction -0.06468 0.06006 238.98943 -1.077 0.28260
P1Pos_P2Neg_Interaction 0.03214 0.13254 238.75444 0.242 0.80863
P1Neg_P2Pos_Interaction -0.05661 0.09975 238.66545 -0.568 0.57090
P1PosBehv 0.01979 0.17187 238.98704 0.115 0.90842
P2PosBehv 0.17714 0.21604 235.16509 0.820 0.41309
P1NegMood 0.99202 0.10152 238.92286 9.772 < 2e-16 ***
P2NegMood 0.36800 0.16143 238.97690 2.280 0.02351 *
P1P2_PosInteraction:we 0.07422 0.02824 238.56740 2.629 0.00913 **
we:P1P2_NegInteraction -0.02988 0.03572 235.01317 -0.837 0.40370
we:P1Pos_P2Neg_Interaction -0.03587 0.04452 238.92619 -0.806 0.42127
we:P1Neg_P2Pos_Interaction 0.01281 0.03132 226.02725 0.409 0.68281
we:P1PosBehv -0.03005 0.04418 236.58848 -0.680 0.49714
we:P2PosBehv -0.05756 0.04998 232.58868 -1.152 0.25064
we:P1NegMood -0.06007 0.03890 236.83481 -1.544 0.12388
we:P2NegMood 0.11310 0.06570 238.98525 1.722 0.08645 .
---
Signif. codes: 0 '***' 0.001 '**' 0.01 '*' 0.05 '.' 0.1 ' ' 1
fit warnings:
fixed-effect model matrix is rank deficient so dropping 8 columns / coefficients
View Code
# calculate R-squaredr.squaredGLMM(lm_model)
R2m R2c
[1,] 0.8007224 0.9235177
Check Model Fit
View Code
residual <-resid(lm_model)plot(fitted(lm_model), resid(lm_model))abline(h =0, col ="red")qqnorm(residual)qqline(residual)
Residual vs. Fitted Plot
Normal Q-Q Plot
View Code
shapiro.test(residual)
Shapiro-Wilk normality test
data: residual
W = 0.95632, p-value = 5.345e-07
Results
Significant Results of the Moderating Effect of We-words on Moods/Behaviors on Conflict Intensity
Multi-Regression Model
Estimates
Standard Error
P-Value
Partner 1 and Negative Moods
1.04
0.10
<0.001
Partner 1 Positive Behaviors and Partner 2 Positive Behaviors
-0.28
0.13
0.04
Partner 1 and 2 Positive Behaviors Moderated by We-words
Figure 1. Interaction Effects of Partner 1 and 2 Positive Behaviors on Conflict Intensity with Log-Transformed Scales
View Code
moderation_plot <-interact_plot(lm_model, pred = P1P2_PosInteraction, modx = we,data = all_vars_withInteract,modx.values =seq(min(all_vars_withInteract$we), max(all_vars_withInteract$we), length.out =2),modx.labels =c("Low", "High"),x.label ="Both Partner 1 and 2 in Positive Behaviors",y.label ="Conflict Intensity",legend.main ="Frequency of We-Words")moderation_plot +theme_linedraw()
Figure 2. Moderation Effects of We-Words on Partner 1 and 2 Positive Behaviors on Conflict Intensity with Log-Transformed Scales
Individual negative mood states in partners are positively associated with conflict intensity.
Positive relationship behaviors in both partners during conflict are negatively correlated with conflict intensity.
The use of We-words moderates the relationship between partners’ positive relationship behaviors and conflict intensity, leading to an increase in conflict intensity.
Discussion
Results indicate that each partner’s negative mood, when considered separately, significantly increased conflict intensity - though the interaction effect of both partners’ negative moods did not significantly affect conflict intensity (p = 0.45).
Positive relationship behaviors during conflict reduced conflict intensity.
Contrary to previous research, the use of We-words did not enhance the buffering effect of positive behaviors; instead, it increased conflict intensity.
Limitations include short duration of conflicts (M = 89.7 seconds, SD = 40.8 seconds) within the 3-minute audio recordings and overall low conflict intensity ratings (M = 8.01, SD = 13.82) on a scale of 0 to 100.
Future research could explore how couples’ mood states and behaviors during conflict influence their emotional recovery post-conflict, moderated by variables such as relationship closeness.
Acknowledgements
Special thanks to my mentor Kayla Carta and Dr. Adela Timmons for their invaluable guidance and feedback throughout this project. Thank you to Jacqueline Duong, Sierra Walters, Alyssa Carrasco, and Daniela Romero for their support during the internship and their contributions in the lab.
Footnotes
Christensen, A., & Shenk, J. L. (1991). Communication, Conflict, and Psychological Distance in Nondistressed, Clinic, and Divorcing Couples. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59(3), 458–463. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.59.3.458↩︎
Kiecolt-Glaser, J. K., & Newton, T. L. (2001). Marriage and Health: His and Hers. Psychological Bulletin, 127(4), 472–503. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.127.4.472↩︎
Gottman, J. M. (2024). What Predicts Divorce? : The Relationship Between Marital Processes and Marital Outcomes / John M. Gottman. (First edition.). Routledge.↩︎
Gottman, J. M., Coan, J., Carrere, S., & Swanson, C. (1998). Predicting Marital Happiness and Stability from Newlywed Interactions. Journal of Marriage and Family, 60(1), 5–22. https://doi.org/10.2307/353438↩︎
Johnson, M. D., Cohan, C. L., Davila, J., Lawrence, E., Rogge, R. D., Karney, B. R., Sullivan, K. T., & Bradbury, T. N. (2005). Problem-Solving Skills and Affective Expressions as Predictors of Change in Marital Satisfaction. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 73(1), 15–27. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.73.1.15↩︎
Kim, H. K., Capaldi, D. M., & Crosby, L. (2007). Generalizability of Gottman and Colleagues’ Affective Process Models of Couples’ Relationship Outcomes. Journal of marriage and the family, 69(1), 55–72. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1741- 3737.2006.00343.x↩︎
Seider, B. H., Hirschberger, G., Nelson, K. L., & Levenson, R. W. (2009). We can work it out: age differences in relational pronouns, physiology, and behavior in marital conflict. Psychology and aging, 24(3), 604–613. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016950↩︎
Rizkalla, L., Wertheim, E. H., & Hodgson, L. K. (2008). The roles of emotion management and perspective taking in individuals’ conflict management styles and disposition to forgive. Journal of Research in Personality, 42(6), 1594– 1601. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2008.07.014↩︎